Impulsividad, funciones ejecutivas y aprendizaje: Una relación para reflexionar

Palabras clave: Impulsividad, Funciones Ejecutivas, Aprendizaje Auto-regulado

Resumen

La impulsividad subclínica, es definida como un rasgo estable de la personalidad en el cual los individuos que la manifiestan presentan conductas de riesgo y una toma de decisiones deficiente. La presencia de impulsividad se asocia a un desempeño subóptimo en diversos procesos cognitivos, como la memoria de trabajo, la flexibilidad cognitiva y la inhibición, procesos que facilitan la organización y la regulación de la conducta, por lo que impactan de forma negativa en distintos ámbitos de la vida cotidiana del individuo, como el laboral, el social y el académico. El presente trabajo tiene como propósito hacer una revisión narrativa del concepto de impulsividad y el funcionamiento ejecutivo en relación con el aprendizaje en estudiantes de nivel superior.

Descargas

La descarga de datos todavía no está disponible.

Citas

American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales DSM-5. Madrid: Médica Panamericana.

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559.

Basar, K., Sesia, T., Groenewegen, H., Steinbusch, H., Visser-Vandewalle, V., & Temel, Y. (2010). Nucleo accumbens and impulsivity. Prog Neurobiol., 92(4), 533-57.

Berlin, H., Rolls, E., & Kischka, U. (2004). Impulsivity, time perception, emotion, and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 127(5), 1108-26.

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. (2011). Relations between executive functions and academic achivement from ages 5-17 in a large representative national sample. Learning and individual differences, 21, 327-336.

Brooks, S., Wiemerslage, L., Burch, K., Maiorana, S., Cocolas, E., Schiöth, H. B., . . . Stein, D. J. (2017). The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology (234), 1911-1921.

Bryant, F. B., Yarnold, P. R., & Grimm, L. G. (1996). Toward a measure model of the Affect Intensity Measure: A three factor structure. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 223-247.

Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children. Neuron, 33(2), 301-311.

Correa, A., Triviño, M., Perez-Dueñas, C., Acosta, A., & Lupiañez, J. (2010). Temporal preparation, response inhibition and impulsivity. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 222-228.

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and individual differences, 13, 653-65.

Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci., 38(9), 571-578.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. (64), 135-168.

Ellingson, J. M., Fleming, K. A., Verges, A., Bartholow, B. D., & Sher, K. J. (2014). Working memory as a moderator of impulsivity and alcohol involvement: testing the cognitive-motivational theory of alcohol use with prospective and working memory updating data. Addict Behav., 39(11), 1622-1631.

Eysenck, S. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1977). The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 57-68.

Fallgatter, A. J., & Herrmann, M. J. (2001). Electrophysiological assessment of impulsive behavior in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 39(3), 328-333.

Follmer, D. J., & Sperling, R. A. (2016). The mediating role of metacognition in the relationship between executive function and self-regulated learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology (DOI:10.1111/bjep.12123), 1-17.

Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett, C., Owen, R., & Schroeter, K. (2003). Enhancing third grade student' mathematical problem solving with self-regulated learning strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 306-315.

Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology, 31(3-5), 373-385.

Garavan, H., Hester, R., Murphy, K., Fassbender, C., & Kelly, C. (2006). Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control. Brain Research, 1105(1), 130-42.

Garner, J. K. (2009). Conceptualizing the relations between executive functions and self-regulated learning. The Journal of Psychology, 143(3), 405-426.

Lijffijt, M., Bekker, E., Quik, E., Bakker, J., Kenemans, J., & Verbaten, M. (2004). Differences between low and high trait impulsivity are not Referencias

American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales DSM-5. Madrid: Médica Panamericana.

Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255(5044), 556-559.

Basar, K., Sesia, T., Groenewegen, H., Steinbusch, H., Visser-Vandewalle, V., & Temel, Y. (2010). Nucleo accumbens and impulsivity. Prog Neurobiol., 92(4), 533-57.

Berlin, H., Rolls, E., & Kischka, U. (2004). Impulsivity, time perception, emotion, and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 127(5), 1108-26.

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. (2011). Relations between executive functions and academic achivement from ages 5-17 in a large representative national sample. Learning and individual differences, 21, 327-336.

Brooks, S., Wiemerslage, L., Burch, K., Maiorana, S., Cocolas, E., Schiöth, H. B., . . . Stein, D. J. (2017). The impact of cognitive training in substance use disorder: the effect of working memory training on impulse control in methamphetamine users. Psychopharmacology (234), 1911-1921.

Bryant, F. B., Yarnold, P. R., & Grimm, L. G. (1996). Toward a measure model of the Affect Intensity Measure: A three factor structure. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 223-247.

Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children. Neuron, 33(2), 301-311.

Correa, A., Triviño, M., Perez-Dueñas, C., Acosta, A., & Lupiañez, J. (2010). Temporal preparation, response inhibition and impulsivity. Brain and Cognition, 73(3), 222-228.

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and individual differences, 13, 653-65.

Dajani, D. R., & Uddin, L. Q. (2015). Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci., 38(9), 571-578.

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. (64), 135-168.

Ellingson, J. M., Fleming, K. A., Verges, A., Bartholow, B. D., & Sher, K. J. (2014). Working memory as a moderator of impulsivity and alcohol involvement: testing the cognitive-motivational theory of alcohol use with prospective and working memory updating data. Addict Behav., 39(11), 1622-1631.

Eysenck, S. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1977). The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 57-68.

Fallgatter, A. J., & Herrmann, M. J. (2001). Electrophysiological assessment of impulsive behavior in healthy subjects. Neuropsychologia, 39(3), 328-333.

Follmer, D. J., & Sperling, R. A. (2016). The mediating role of metacognition in the relationship between executive function and self-regulated learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology (DOI:10.1111/bjep.12123), 1-17.

Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86, 186-204.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett, C., Owen, R., & Schroeter, K. (2003). Enhancing third grade student' mathematical problem solving with self-regulated learning strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 306-315.

Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development. Journal of Neurocytology, 31(3-5), 373-385.

Garavan, H., Hester, R., Murphy, K., Fassbender, C., & Kelly, C. (2006). Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control. Brain Research, 1105(1), 130-42.

Garner, J. K. (2009). Conceptualizing the relations between executive functions and self-regulated learning. The Journal of Psychology, 143(3), 405-426.

Lijffijt, M., Bekker, E., Quik, E., Bakker, J., Kenemans, J., & Verbaten, M. (2004). Differences between low and high trait impulsivity are not associated with differences in inhibitory motor control. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8(1), 25-32.

Logan, G. D., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8(1), 60-64.

Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. (2011). Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 1-17.

Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci., 21(1), 8-14.

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive function: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology (41), 49-100.

Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological bulletin, 126(2), 220-246.

Panwar, K., Rutherford, H., Mencl, W. E., Lacadie, C. M., Potenza, M. N., & Mayes, L. C. (2014). Differential associations between impulsivity and risk-taking and brain activations underlying working memory in adolescents. Addictive Behaviors, 39(11), 1606-1621.

Peng, J., Mo, L., Huang, P., & Zhou, Y. (2017). The effects of working memory training on improving fluid intelligence of children during early childhood. Cognitive Development (43), 224-234.

Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannneta, J. M., Yang, W., & Hurt, H. (2011). Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1119-1133.

Smith, G., & Cyders, M. (2016). Integrating affect and impulsivity: The role of positive and negative urgency in substance use risk. Drug and alcohol dependence, 163, S3-S12.

Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman-Nutley, S., Bihlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106-113.

Vázquez-Moreno, A., González-Garrido, A., & Ramos-Loyo, J. (2019). Delayed response improves inhibitory control in low- and high-impulsivity adolescents: effects of emotional contexts. International Journal of Psychological Studies, 11(2), 42-53.

Vosniadou, S., Pnevmatikos, D., & Makris, N. (2018). The role of executive function in the construction and employment of scientific and mathematical concepts that require conceptual change learning. Revue NeuroEducation Journal, 5(2), 62-72.

Zelazo, P. D., Blair, C. B., & Willoughby, M. T. (2016). Executive functions: Implications for education (NCER 2017-2000). Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Research.

Publicado
2022-01-05
Cómo citar
Vázquez-Moreno, A. (2022). Impulsividad, funciones ejecutivas y aprendizaje: Una relación para reflexionar. Boletín Científico De La Escuela Superior Atotonilco De Tula, 9(17), 32-37. https://doi.org/10.29057/esat.v9i17.8157